• Song sparrows shuffle and repeat to keep

    From ScienceDaily@1:317/3 to All on Wed Jan 26 21:30:42 2022
    Song sparrows shuffle and repeat to keep their audience listening
    Playlist is switched up and remembered for at least 30 minutes

    Date:
    January 26, 2022
    Source:
    Duke University
    Summary:
    Biologists have found an animal for the first time that communicates
    with the complexity of human language: song sparrows. According to
    a new study, male song sparrows memorize a 30-minute long playlist
    of their recently belted tunes and use that information to curate
    both their current playlist and the next one. The findings suggest
    that song sparrows deliberately shuffle and repeat their songs
    possibly to keep a female's attention.



    FULL STORY ==========================================================================
    The tweets of a little song sparrow and its 'bird brain' are a lot more
    complex and akin to human language than anyone realized. A new study finds
    that male sparrows deliberately shuffle and mix their song repertoire
    possibly as a way to keep it interesting for their female audience.


    ==========================================================================
    The research, from the lab of Stephen Nowicki, Duke University professor
    of biology and member of the Duke Institute for Brain Sciences, and
    colleagues at the University of Miami, shows that singing males keep
    track of the order of their songs and how often each one is sung for up
    to 30 minutes so they can curate both their current playlist and the
    next one. The findings appear in Proceedings of the Royal Society Bon
    January 26.

    Song sparrows are a common songbird throughout North America, but only
    males sing. They use their song to defend their turf and court mates.

    When wooing, song sparrows belt up to 12 different two-second songs,
    a repertoire that can take nearly 30 minutes to get through, since they
    repeat the same song several times before going on to the next track. In addition to varying the number of repeats, males also shuffle the order
    of their tunes each time they sing their discography. However, a big
    unknown had been whether males change up their song order and repeats
    by accident or by design.

    To get some data on whether or not the birds intentionally shuffle and
    mix their tunes, Nowicki's long-time collaborator William Searcy, the
    Maytag Professor of Ornithology in Biology at the University of Miami,
    loaded up the recording gear, trekked out to the backwoods of northwest Pennsylvania, set up mics pointed to the trees and patiently waited for
    five hours a day.

    Nowicki says that fieldwork like this isn't for everyone, "I would never
    use the word boring, because it's relaxing if you like being out in
    the field and it's a nice day and you've got your parabolic microphone
    and you're pointing it at a song sparrow for hours. Some people would
    find that boring. I and certainly Bill would find that meditatively
    relaxing. The only thing that happens is sometimes your arm gets tired."
    After recording the full suite of songs from more than 30 birds, the team
    pored over visual spectrographs of the trills and analyzed how often each
    song was sung and in what order. The first clue that males keep tabs on
    their tweets to avoid repetition was that much like a Spotify playlist,
    males generally sing through their full repertoire before repeating
    a song.



    ==========================================================================
    The researchers also found that the more a sparrow sang a given song,
    the longer he took to get back to that song, possibly to build up hype
    and novelty once that song was played again. For example, if a male sang
    Song A 10 times in a row, he'd sing even more renditions of his other
    songs before returning to Song A again. Alternatively, if Song A was
    only warbled three times during a set, then a male song sparrow might
    recite a shorter rendition of the rest of his repertoire in order to
    return to the still novel and underplayed Song A.

    Taken together, these findings demonstrate that song sparrows possess
    an extremely rare talent with an equally uncommon name: "long-distance dependencies." It means that what a male song sparrow sings in the moment depends on what he sang as much as 30 minutes ago. That's a 360 times
    larger memory capacity than the previous record holder, the canary, who
    can only juggle about five seconds worth of song information in this way.

    While impressive, the implications from this work for humans are less
    clear. It does suggest that the order of words in human language, which
    is similarly impacted by long-distance dependencies may not be as unique
    as once thought.

    It remains to be seen whether better shuffling ability gives males an
    advantage at finding love. Perhaps females maintain interest in a mate
    who mixes it up more, and are less likely to sneak off with another
    male. As with daytime talk shows, paternity tests are a good proxy for
    monogamy in birds, so counting how many chicks are sired by a female's
    nest mate versus another bird in the neighborhood may be a future project
    for Nowicki's team.

    For now, Nowicki emphasizes it's just speculation whether these shuffling
    song sparrows give Spotify a run for their money to keep a female's
    interest, but does highlight our similar approach at the gym.

    "You've got your playlist for running and the reason you've got that is
    because running is kind of boring. You know that these 10 songs are going
    to keep you motivated, but if you are going to run for 20 songs long,
    why not shuffle it so the next time you don't hear the same songs in
    the same order?" Support for the research came from the Office of the
    Provost, Duke University, and the Robert E. Maytag Professor Research
    Fund of the University of Miami.

    ========================================================================== Story Source: Materials provided by Duke_University. Original written
    by Dan Vahaba. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


    ========================================================================== Journal Reference:
    1. William A. Searcy, Jill Soha, Susan Peters, Stephen Nowicki. Long-
    distance dependencies in birdsong syntax. Proceedings of the Royal
    Society B: Biological Sciences, 2022; 289 (1967) DOI: 10.1098/
    rspb.2021.2473 ==========================================================================

    Link to news story: https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2022/01/220126122443.htm

    --- up 7 weeks, 4 days, 7 hours, 13 minutes
    * Origin: -=> Castle Rock BBS <=- Now Husky HPT Powered! (1:317/3)